Prof. Stanislas Dehaene is a neuroscientist and the below is a lecture he gave aimed at teachers about how the brain learns to read. It was interesting to see "learning to read" from the perspective of a scientist rather than an educator. This isn't a new lecture (it is about 10 years old) but it is amazing what scientists can now observe using technology that can then be used to inform best practice in the classroom.
I have included some notes that summarise Prof. Stanislas Dehaene's lecture below. I will use these notes to help inform my intervention design in the coming weeks. I have also included at the end of this blog a graphic by "Learning Matters" that has been created based on Prof. Stanislas Dehaene's work as a neuroscientist.
Here are some of the key points he makes in the video:
From the brain's point of view, learning to read consists of:
- First, recognising the letters and how they combine into written words (the blue and the red)
- Second, connecting them to the brain systems for coding of speech sounds and for meaning (the orange and the green).
Reading starts in your brain like any other visual stimulation, in the general visual areas of the brain, but then very quickly moves into an area which concerns the recognition of the written word.
Professor Dehaene calls this area the brain's "letter box" because it is where we store our knowledge of letters.
From there it's an explosion of activity into at least two brain networks; one that concerns the meaning of the words, and another that concerns the pronunciation and the articulation of the words.
When first you went to school to learn to read, your brain already had a very sophisticated spoken language system. It also had a very sophisticated visual system, but it needed to create an interface with the visual word form area, the brain's letter box.
Reading consists essentially of creating an interface between your vision system in your brain and your language system, your spoken language system.
Your brain has areas that are shared between spoken language and written language. And these areas are in your brain before you learn to read. They already existed when you were a very young child.
Brain is changed by reading
Everyone's brain anatomy is changed by learning to read, and Professor Dehaene and his colleagues have managed to make a complete map of the brain areas that have been changed by reading.
The first major change is making the "letter box area” active. It is not active in people who haven't learnt to read.
Learning to read is to a large extent the capacity to attend to the individual phonemes of speech and to attribute them different letters.
What predicts how well a child will learn to read
The predictors of learning to read in young children are:
- How well they know phonics - the understanding of the sound systems of language
- How large is the size of their spoken vocabulary. If they know a large number of words, they will learn to read faster.
"We can help children learn to read, way in advance, by enhancing their vocabulary and their sound system of language at the age of three, four and five - even if we don’t start to teach reading until the age of six or seven, as some European countries do" - says Professor Dehaene.
Phonics versus whole word reading
We now understand better the question of phonics versus whole-word reading.
Should we teach the whole-word level, or should we really teach every single letter and the pronunciation?
The answer is clear. Professor Dehaene says, "whole-word reading is a myth". The brain processes every single letter and does not look at the whole word shape.
Teaching letter to sound correspondence is therefore essential. It is the fastest way to acquire reading and comprehension.
Preschools should be preparing children for learning to read and phonics exercises can help a lot.
"The brain process' every single letter and does not look at the whole word shape. Teaching letter sound correspondence is therefore essential. It is the fastest way to acquire reading and comprehension." - says Professor Dehaene.
Brains are the same all around the world
We understand now a lot about reading, and we understand that in all cultures, there's not much variability. We all have the same brain mechanisms – they are very universal.
Reading always requires specialising of visual system for the shape of letters and connecting them to speech sounds. Even in Chinese (which has characters, rather than letters).
Nga mihi nui!!
No comments:
Post a Comment